- انصاریسامانی، حبیب؛ و همکاران، (1394). «مقدمهای بر اقتصاد رفتاری: مفهوم، روششناسی و شیوههای استخراج ترجیحات». اقتصاد تطبیقی، 2 (1): 37-69.
- تیموری، عباد؛ و همکاران، (1396). «نقد انتخاب عقلانی از منظر رویکردهای رقیب: اقتصاد رفتاری، آزمایشگاهی و علوم مغزی». پژوهشهای اقتصادی ایران، 22 (7): 43-1.
- رهبر، فرهاد؛ و همکاران، (1392). «اقتصاددانان رفتاری و نظریههای آنها». برنامهریزی و بودجه، 18 (1): 165-133.
- Ariely, D., (2008). Predictably irrational. New York: Harper Audio New York.
- Ansari Samani, H. et al., (2014). “An Introduction to Behavioral Economics: Concept, Methodology and Methods of Extracting Preferences”. Comparative Economics, 2(1): 37-69. (In Persian)
- Aysola, J.; Tahirovic, E.; Troxel, A.B.; Asch, D.A.; Gangemi, K.; Hodlofski, A. T. et al., (2018). “A randomized controlled trial of opt-in versus opt-out enrollment into a diabetes behavioral intervention”. Am J Health Promot, 32(3): 745–52.
- Biddle, N., (2021). “Behavioral economics and the covid-induced education crisis”. OECD education working paper, 254 (11): 1-45.
- Bickel, W. K.; Moody, L. & Higgins, S. T., (2016). Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change. Prev Med.
- Biran, A.; Schmidt, W-P.; Varadharajan, KS.; Rajaraman. D.; Kumar, R.; Greenland, K. et al., (2014). “Effect of a behaviour-change intervention on handwashing with soap in India (SuperAmma): a cluster-randomised trial”. Lancet Glob Health, 2(3): e145–e154154.
- Chapman, G. B.; Li, M.; Colby, H., & Yoon, H., (2010). “Opting in vs opting out of influenza vaccination”. JAMA, 304(1): 43–4.
- Cifuentes, F. J., (2020). “The importance of behavioral economics during covid-19”. Journal of economics and behavioral studies, 12(3): 70-74.
- Coleman, S., (2007). “The Minnesota income tax compliance experiment: replication of the social norms experiment”. Available at SSRN 1393292.
- Congdon, W. J., & Shankar, M., (2018). “The role of behavioral economics in evidence-based policymaking”. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 678(1): 81–92.
- Dillard, A. J.; McCaul, K. D. & Klein, W. M., (2006). “Unrealistic optimism in smokers: Implications for smoking myth endorsement and selfprotective motivation”. J Health Commun, 11(S1): 93–102. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.10185.
- Finucane, M.L.; Alhakami, A.; Slovic, P. & Johnson, S. M., (2000). “The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits”. J Behav Decis Mak, 13(1): 1–17.
- Fogg, B., (2009). “A behavior model for persuasive design”. In: Proceedings of the 4th international Conference on Persuasive Technology: 40. ACM.
- Gallagher, S., (2020). “Coronavirus: How to Ensure your Relationships Survive Self-Isolation”. Digital News Brand Independent. Information website: https://www.independent.co.uk (accessed April 13, 2020).
- Halpern, S. D.; French, B.; Small, D. S.; Saulsgiver, K.; Harhay, M. O. & Audrain-McGovern, J. et al., (2015). “Randomized trial of four financialincentive programs for smoking cessation”. N Engl J Med, 372: 2108–17.
- Haushofer, J. y. & Metcalf, J., (2020). “Combining behavioral economics and infectious disease epidemiology to mitigate the COVID-19 outbreak”.
- Hearne, R. R. & Salinas, Z. M., (2002). “The use of choice experiments in the analysis of tourist preferences for ecotourism development in Costa rica”. Journal of environmental management, 65(2): 153-163.
- Hensher, D. A.; Rose, J. & Bertoia, T., (2007). “The implications on willingness to pay of a stochastic treatment of attribute processing in stated choice studies”. Transportation Research, 43(2): 73–89. doi:10.1016/j.tre.2005.07.006.
- Hussam, R.; Rabbani, A.; Reggiani, G. & Rigol, N., (2017). “Habit formation and rational addiction: a field experiment in handwashing”. Harvard Business School BGIE Unit working: 18-30.
- John, L. K.; Loewenstein, G.; Troxel, A. B.; Norton, L.; Fassbender, J. E. & Volpp, K. G., (2011). “Financial incentives for extended weight loss: a randomized”. Controlled trial. J Gen Intern Med, 26(6): 621–6.
- Kahneman, D.; Slovic, S. P. Slovic, P. & Tversky, A., (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press.
- Kliger, D., (2020). Economic behavior and behavioral economics at times of covid-19 pandemic, mind and society. Springer: 1-8.
- Kumar, B., (2020). “Covid-19 pandemic and the role of behavioral economics”. MPRA: 1-12.
- Kunu, S. & Duran, S., (2021). “Understanding covid-19 virus pandemic in terms of behavioral economics in terms of how people think and learn”. Economi: 111-118.
- Kwok, Y. L. A.; Gralton, J. & McLaws, M. L., (2015). “Face touching: A frequent habit that has implications for hand hygiene”. American Journal of Infection Control, 43(2): 112–114.
- Laibson, D., (1997). “Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting”. Q J Econ. 112(2): 443–78.
- Linnemayr, S.; Stecher, C. & Mukasa, B., (2017), “Behavioral economic incentives to improve adherence to antiretroviral medication”. AIDS (London, England), 31(5): 719.
- Loewenstein, G.; Asch, D. A.; Friedman, J. Y., Melichar, L. A. & Volpp, K. G., (2012). “Can behavioural economics make us healthier?”. BMJ, 344: e3482.
- Loewenstein, G.; Brennan, T. & Volpp, K. G., (2007). “Asymmetric paternalism to improve health behaviors”. JAMA, 298(20): 2415–7.
- Luoto, J. & Carman, K. G., (2014). Behavioral economics guidelines with applications for health interventions. Washington: Inter American Development Bank.
- Matjasko, J. L. Cawley, J. H.; Baker-Goering, M. M. & Yokum, D. V., (2016). “Applying behavioral economics to public health policy: illustrative examples and promising directions”. Am J Prev Med., 50(5): S13–S1919.
- McFadden, D., (1973). Conditional logit analysis of qualitative choice behavior. New York: Academic Press.
- O’Donoghue, T. & Rabin, M., (1999). “Doing it now or later”. Am Econ Rev., 89(1): 103–24.
- Ortega, D. L.; Wang, H. H.; Wu, L. & Olynk, N. J., (2011). “Modeling heterogeneity in consumer preferences for select food safety attributes in China”. Food Policy, 36(2): 318–324. doi:10.1016/j.foodpol.2010.11.030.
- Peters, E.; Lipkus, I. & Diefenbach, M. A., (2006). “The functions of affect in health communications and in the construction of health preferences”. J Commun, 56: S140–S162162.
- Raafat, R. M.; Chater, N. & Frith, C., (2009). “Herding in humans”. Trends Cognit Sci., 13(10): 420–8.
- Rahbar, F. et al., (2012). “Behavioral economists and their theories”. Planning and Budgeting, 18(1): 133-165, (In Persian)
- Samuelson, W. & Zeckhauser, R., (1988). “Status quo bias in decision making”. J Risk Uncertain, 1(1): 7–59.
- Schwarz, N., (2011). “Feelings-as-information theory”. In: Van Lange P, Kruglanski A, Higgins ET, editors. Handbook of theories of social psychology, Thousand Oaks: Sage: 289–308.
- Slovic, P. & Peters, E., (2006). “Risk perception and affect”. Curr Dir Psychol Sci., 15(6): 322–5.
- Taymoori, E. et al., (2016). “Criticism of rational choice from the perspective of competing approaches: behavioral economics, laboratory and brain science”. Iran's economic research, 22(73): 1-43, (In Persian).
- Thaler, R. H. & Sunstein, C. R., (2008). “Nudge: improving decisions about health, wealth and happiness”. The Social Science, New Haven: Yale University Press.
- Thaler, R. H., (2016). “Behavioral economics: past, present, and future”. American Economic Review, 106(7).
- Thaler, R. H., (2018). “From cashews to nudges: The evolution of behavioral economics”. American Economic Review, 108(6).
- Train, K., (2003). Discrete choice methods with simulation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D., (1979). “Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk”. Econometrica, 47(2): 263–91.
-Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D., (1974). “Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases”. Science, 185(4157).
- Van der Pol, M. & Cairns, J., (2011). “Descriptive validity of alternative intertemporal models for health outcomes: an axiomatic test”. Health Econ., 20(7): 770–82.
- Van Der Pol, M.; Hennessy, D. & Manns, B., (2017). “The role of time and risk preferences in adherence to physician advice on health behavior change”. Eur J Health Econ., 18(3): 373–86.
- Voj-a-cek, O. & Pec-akov-a, I., (2010). “Comparison of discrete choice models for economic environmental research”. Prague Economic Papers, 1: 35–53. doi: 10.18267/j.pep.363.
- Watson, J.; Dreibelbis, R.; Aunger, R.; Deola, C.; King, K. & Long, S. et al., (2019). “Child’s play: harnessing play and curiosity motives to improve child handwashing in a humanitarian setting”. Int J Hyg Environ Health., 222(2): 177–82.
- Weinstein, N. D.; Marcus, S. E. & Moser, R. P., (2005). “Smokers’ unrealistic optimism about their risk”. Tob control, 14(1): 55–9.
- Weinstein, N. D., (1980). “Unrealistic optimism about future life events”. J Pers Soc Psychol, 39(5): 806.
- Weinstein, N. D., (1987). “Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: conclusions from a community-wide sample”. J Behav Med., 10(5): 481–500.
- White, J. S. & Dow, W. H., (2015). “Intertemporal choices for health”. In: Roberto Ch A, Kawachi I, editors, Behavioral economics and public health, Oxford: Oxford University Press; 27: 62.
- Wilder-Smith, A. & Freedman, D., (2020). “Isolation, quarantine, social distancing and community containment: pivotal role for old style public health measures in the novel coronavirus (2019-nCoV) outbreak”. J Travel Med., 27(2).
- Wu, C. et al., (2020). “The dynamics of trust before, during and after the covid-19 outbreak”. Canadian institutes of health research, Information website: https://www.csa-scs.ca/.